ОСОБЕННОСТИ ПРЕФИКСАЛЬНОГО ИНВЕНТАРЯ СОВРЕМЕННОГО АНГЛИЙСКОГО ЯЗЫКА

Научная статья
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.18454/RULB.7.29
Выпуск: № 3 (7), 2016
PDF

Аннотация

Статья посвящена системному описанию английских префиксов. Рассматриваются такие свойства как: происхождения, транспортирующий характер, отрицательность, валентность, а также семантические особенности (полисемия, включенность в синонимические ряды). Теоретическая значимость исследования состоит в важности словообразовательного аспекта для установления типологических характеристик языка. Практическая значимость объясняется важностью определения особенностей морфем иностранного языка в целях обучения словообразованию. В основе работы лежит квантитативный метод.

A prefix is generally defined as an affixal morpheme that follows the word-building stem. To characterize the prefixal inventory it is necessary first to estimate it. It can be classified into productive and non-productive.  P.M. Karaschuk characterizes  14 productive prefixes, pointing out also 57 less productive.

In this paper we consider functional and semantic features of prefixes of the modern English language. The theoretical significance is based on the importance of the word-building aspect in estimating typological characteristics of a language. Practical significance is explained by the importance of finding out features of a foreign language morphemes in teaching word-building.

To describe the elements of the prefixal system we will take the criteria described by  O.D. Meshkov (origin, transporting opportunity, negation, valency). 

Origin

While speaking about prefixes it is important to mention an important role of borrowings. 6 out of 14 productive prefixes described by P.M. Karaschuk are borrowed (that makes up 42,5%). The main sources of borrowings are Latin (83,3% prefixes)  and Greek (16,7%).  Among the less productive prefixes the share of the borrowings is much higher coming up to 88,5% (50 prefixes out of 57). The comparative data are given in the chart:

30-08-2016 10-19-13

Fig. 1

 

Transporting opportunity

According to numerous researchers English prefixes are generally non-transportive. Their main function is, accordingly, to change the lexical rather than grammatical meaning of the productive stem.  Only  3 (21,4%)  out of 14 productive prefixes are transportive. .

However, V.I.Shkarupin comes up with another point  of view. He points out two groups of English transportive prefixes:

  1. verb-forming from nouns and adjectives: be-, de-, dis-, en\in-, inter-, out-, over-, re-, un;
  2. adjective-forming from nouns,  word combinations and participles: anti-, inter-, intra-, multi-, over-, poly-, post-, pre-, pro-, sub-, super-, supra-, un-, under-.

Still it is important to consider the fact that many (however, not all) of the above mentioned prefixes form words of a different part of speech in case of the simultaneous conversion. For example, it is possible to form an adjective  underwater by prefixation with  under- from the stem water. However, it will be the conversed stem of an adjective (as there is a word combination water transport in English).

Negation

This feature is typical for a huge share of prefixes (50% of the data). It makes up an important difference between prefixal and suffixal systems of the English language. A huge number of negative prefixes enhances the important role of prefixation as a way of changing lexical meaning.

Valency

Most productive English prefixes have single valency (64,2%). Single-valency prefixes in the English language are of two types: verbal and adjectival. It is evident, that many English nouns contain prefixes, but according to P.M. Karaschuk these are mostly derivatives from prefixal adjectives and verbs, so there is no act of adding a prefix to a noun stem in them. Out of single-valency productive prefixes  33% are adjectival , 67% - verbal. The chart below illustrates the data:

30-08-2016 10-19-28

Fig. 2

 

Further on we give the brief description of the basic English prefixes:

un- (adj) – native, non-transportive, negative, single-valency

in- – borrowed, non-transportive, negative, single-valency

non- - borrowed, non-transportive, negative, multi-valency

a- - borrowed, non-transportive, negative, single-valency

self- – native, non-transportive, non-negative, multi-valency

un- (v) – native, non-transportive, negative, single-valency

de- – borrowed, transportive, negative, multi-valency

dis- – borrowed, transportive, negative, single-valency

mis- - native, non-transportive, non-negative, multi-valency

under- – native, non-transportive, non-negative, single-valency

over- – native, non-transportive, non-negative, single-valency

up- – native, non-transportive, non-negative, single-valency

re- -borrowed, non-transportive, non-negative, single-valency

be- - native, transportive, non-negative, multi-valency

Further in the paper we consider the comparison of prefixes basing on the part of speech reference.

The data are to be presented in tables setting the presence of certain features. The following markers were chosen: native (NT), transporting (Т), negative (NG), single-valency (S). The presence of a feature is marked in the tables as 1, the absence as  0.

Adjectival prefixes

The analysis data are presented in the table 1.

 

Table 1

30-08-2016 10-19-51

 

The data above cause the following conclusions:

  1. 2 of the considered prefixes  (40%) are native,  while others are borrowed. It is in place to mention here, that all the borrowed prefixes are synonyms to the native prefix un-;
  2. non-transportive character is typical of adjectival prefixes (100% of the data considered);
  3. 4 prefixes (80% of the selected data) are negative and make up a chain of synonyms;
  4. single-valency is dominating (3 prefixes out of 5), that can be explained by the non-transportive character of adjectival prefixes.

Verbal prefixes

The comparative data are presented in the table 2.

 

Table 2

30-08-2016 10-20-09

 

The given data give way to the following conclusions:

  1. out of the data considered 2\3 are native prefixes. This phenomenon can be explained by the fact many productive prefixes are derived from prepositions
  2. only 3 out of  9 verbal prefixes are transportive, that supports the theory of lexical rather than grammatical meaning of prefixes
  3. 3 prefixes are negative and make up a chain of synonyms (while 2 of them are borrowings)
  4. most prefixes are single-valency, which can be explained by their non-transportive nature.

Further on the paper considers the comparison of adjectival and verbal prefixes. Under consideration is the total of productive affixes and the share of characteristics: native (NT), transporting (Т), negative (NG), single-valency (S).

 

Table 3

30-08-2016 10-20-27

 

As the comparison shows there is difference in verbal and adjectival prefixes. Most adjectival prefixes are borrowed, while most verbal prefixes are native. Adjectival prefixes unlike verbal cannot be transportive. Negation is typical to most adjectival prefixes (80%), while in verbal prefixes it accounts for  33,3% cases. As far as valency is concerned the characteristics are similar: in both cases most prefixes are single-valency (60% and 66,7% accordingly).   

Speaking of semantics of prefixes it is important to mention that prefixes are more independent than suffixes. It can be explained by the fact that a lot of prefixes have originated from independent part of speech (such as prepositions).

O.D. Meshkov, I.V. Orlovskaya point out the following groups of prefixal meanings:

  • preceding
  • following
  • surplus
  • lack
  • incompleteness
  • negation
  • repetition
  • deprivation
  • combination
  • counteraction
  • mistake.

Further in the paper, basing upon the data above and practical material obtained while analyzing lexicographical data, we give the description of semantic peculiarities of verbal and adjectival prefixes. .

Semantic comparison of adjectival prefixes is given further in a table 4.

 

Table 4

30-08-2016 10-22-52

 

As the table shows polysemy is typical of two prefixes (in-, self-, with 2 meanings in both cases).

The only meaning possessed by more than 1 prefix is negation. It is typical of  such prefixes as un-, non-, a- ant with a stronger shade — of  in-. The above mentioned prefixes form a chain of synonyms, where the native prefix  un- predominates.  Other borrowed prefixes are strictly limited by collocations.

Verbal prefixes are also considered further as a table 5.

 

Table 5

30-08-2016 10-23-43

 

For  6 of the prefixes given in the table  above polysemy is typical. These are the prefixes de- (2 meanings), dis- (3 meanings), mis – (2 meanings), under- (2 meanings), over- (2 meanings), be- (3 meanings)  

As the table above shows,  3 prefixes form a chain of synonyms «reverse action» (prefixes un-, de-, dis), with the dominating native prefix un-. 3 prefixes also have  the meanings of deprivation and direction, but they don't form chains of synonyms (in the first case due to other meanings of prefixes, in the second case due to different direction of action).

In general while describing the semantics of English prefixes the following conclusions can be made:

1) polysemy is more typical of verbal prefixes (66%), than of those of adjectives (28%);

2) the most common meanings are negation, reverse action, deprivation and direction.

As the research above shows, prefixes of the modern English language constitute a very particular sub-system of morphemes and should be further researched integrally with other elements. 

Список литературы

  • Бархударов Л.С. Очерки по морфологии современного английского языка. – М., 2009

  • Каращук П.М. Словообразование английского языка. – М.: Высшая школа,1977

  • Мешков О.Д. Словообразование современного английского языка. – М.: Наука, 1975